Skip banner
HomeSourcesHow Do I?OverviewHelp
Return To Search FormFOCUS
Search Terms: fuel, efficiency, standards

Document ListExpanded ListKWICFULL format currently displayed

Previous Document Document 49 of 175. Next Document

Copyright 2000 The Houston Chronicle Publishing Company  
The Houston Chronicle

August 13, 2000, Sunday 2 STAR EDITION

SECTION: OUTLOOK; Pg. 4

LENGTH: 1274 words

HEADLINE: Policy based on fear rather than fact is risky business

BYLINE: DAVID ROPEIK; Ropeik is director of risk communication for the Harvard Center for Risk Analysis, in Cambridge, Mass.

BODY:
HUNDREDS of thousands of Americans will die this year, deaths that can be prevented. Millions will get sick with preventable illnesses. Billions of dollars and countless hours of effort will be wasted unnecessarily - all because we're afraid of the wrong things.

In a frenzy of fear, we're pouring millions this summer into protecting ourselves from the West Nile virus, and spending only a fraction of that sum on public education encouraging people to wash their hands, which would eliminate far more disease transmission than killing every mosquito in America.

Public and private spending on the clean-up of hazardous waste is estimated at $ 30 billion a year. Hazardous waste is a real problem, but the number of people whom it puts at risk is actually quite low. Compare that $ 30 billion with only $ 500 million a year on programs to reduce smoking, one of the leading preventable causes of death.

In many areas, science can identify the physical hazards, tell us how many people are likely to be affected by each one, what various mitigations will cost and how effective we can expect them to be. We can rank risks and remedies and put things in perspective. But we don't. Instead, we make policy based more on fear than fact.

This irrational response kills people. In a world of finite resources, we can protect ourselves from only so many things. If we overspend on risks such as pesticides or asbestos, which are real but of relatively low magnitude, we have less to spend on greater threats such as bacterial food poisoning or fossil fuel emissions. As a result, thousands of the people exposed to those higher risks will die.

Bad policy is usually blamed on politics, greed, the media, even the open, manipulable nature of democracy itself. True, these are all factors in a process that often becomes a battle between competing private agendas rather than an informed search for policies to serve the greatest common good. But the principal underlying cause of wasteful choices that seek protection from the wrong is fear.

By definition, fear is more emotional than rational. We fear before we think. Cognitive scientist Joseph LeDoux of New York University identified neural pathways that send information about possible hazards to the amygdala, the fear response center in the ancient core of the brain, before the same information is sent to the cortex, the newer, thinking, rational part of the brain.

But society must be more rational than that. When individual fears become group fears, and when those groups, organized or not, become big enough or visible enough to put pressure on the government to provide protection from less dangerous threats, we can end up with policies that leave many people in the way of harm from higher risks that we're doing less about.

There are some universal perception factors, identified by social psychologist Paul Slovic and others, that make us afraid of the same things and thus tend to turn individual fears into group fears that then foster irrational government policy. Among them are:



Control vs. no control: You normally feel in control when you drive, thus are less afraid. Not so when you're an airplane passenger bumping through turbulence at 30,000 feet.



Immediate/catastrophic vs. chronic: We tend to be more afraid of what can kill a lot of us suddenly and violently, like a plane crash, than, say, lung cancer, which causes hundreds of thousands more deaths, but one at a time, over time.



Natural vs. human-made: We're less afraid of radiation from the sun than of the radiation from power lines and cell phone towers, though the risk from the sun is immensely greater. This one helps explain widespread fear of new technology and chemicals.



Risk vs. benefit: Medicines often have dangerous side effects, but the more we perceive a benefit from the drug, the less we fear its risks.



Imposed vs. voluntary: Nonsmokers are often fearful of tobacco smoke. Smokers usually aren't.



Trust vs. distrust: Experts in the field say this is often the most important risk perception factor, the fulcrum on which the whole seesaw of risk perception rests. Our fears subside if we trust the people informing us about a risk, and if we accept and trust that risk policies are determined in an open and reasonable process. If we don't trust the information or the process, our fears rise, as the Pentagon discovered in the suspicious response of a few service members to its anthrax vaccination program.

How do we make policy-making more rational? There is a model.

Some years ago, the Environmental Protection Agency and the automobile industry declared something of a truce in their war over the science of automobile emissions. Instead of each side spending millions on self-funded research the other side wouldn't accept, they each put in 50 percent of a total $ 6 million to create the Health Effects Institute. HEI was created not to make policy but to give policy-makers credible, trustworthy scientific information on which rational policy could be based. It was to be an impartial scientific review board, outside the government. It appoints panels of scientists, representing their various fields somewhat as a jury represents the community in a trial, so no one with an ax to grind can control the process. HEI's success and influence are growing.

Why not create such an independent, nongovernmental agency - let's call it the Risk Analysis Institute - to provide credible, trustworthy guidance on risks? The institute would rank hazards so we could know which are most likely to occur, classify risks according to their consequences and conduct cost-benefit studies to rank mitigation choices by cost and effectiveness.

It would also identify the range of remaining uncertainties, as well as compare the policies of various agencies, to warn when a policy that reduces risk in one area might increase it in another. For example, federal government standards to increase fuel efficiency reduce pollution, but encourage smaller, lighter vehicles, which are more fuel-efficient but more dangerous for passengers.

Trust is perhaps the most important of all the risk-perception factors; a government agency couldn't establish that trust. An institute outside government might. To that end, it should have as much freedom as possible from the influence of politics. Its funding should come without strings, ideally from a mix of sources with competing agendas but willing to invest in credible, sound science. Funds should also be guaranteed, so no contributor can influence outcomes by threatening to cut off cash. Its scientific work would have to be carried out by professionals chosen for their education and training, their expertise and reputations for integrity, neutrality and open-mindedness - not for their political friends.

Final policy-making decisions would still be made by government agencies, preserving citizens' ability to voice their concerns and use the political process to help shape the outcome.

That means lobbyists, politics, the media and money would still have influence. The messy process of policy-making wouldn't change dramatically. But a Risk Analysis Institute's credible analyses, supporting not a specific policy but rational policy-making in general, would incrementally move government decision-making toward wiser, more informed choices.

Injecting rationality into the process is nothing more than good sense. It's time to create a vehicle to produce credible, reliable science to help develop policy-making that looks beyond our fears to what will do the most good.

The longer we wait, the more we risk.

GRAPHIC: Drawing

TYPE: Editorial Opinion

LOAD-DATE: November 21, 2000




Previous Document Document 49 of 175. Next Document


FOCUS

Search Terms: fuel, efficiency, standards
To narrow your search, please enter a word or phrase:
   
About LEXIS-NEXIS® Academic Universe Terms and Conditions Top of Page
Copyright © 2001, LEXIS-NEXIS®, a division of Reed Elsevier Inc. All Rights Reserved.